プラタナス2 の山 12 月 1 週
◆▲をクリックすると長文だけを表示します。ルビ付き表示

○自由な題名
◎水
○旅
○Sigmund Freud tried to cure(感) 英文のみのページ(翻訳用)
Sigmund Freud tried to cure Viennese women of their neuroses, and Konrad Lorenz made his reputation studying birds, but the two men shared a belief that has become lodged in the popular consciousness. The belief is that we have within us, naturally and spontaneously, a reservoir of aggressive energy. This force, which builds up all by itself, must be periodically drained off -- say, by participating in competitive sports -- lest we explode into violence.
This is an appealing model because it is easy to visualize. It is also false. As an animal behaviorist has written: "All of our present data indicate that fighting behavior among higher mammals, including man, originates in external stimulation and that there is no evidence of spontaneous internal stimulation." Clearly, many people -- and, in fact, whole cultures -- manage quite well without behaving aggressively, and there is no evidence of the inexorable build-up of pressure that this "hydraulic" model would predict.
In 1986, a group of eminent behavioral scientists met in Seville, Spain, to discuss the roots of human aggression and concluded not only that the hydraulic model is inaccurate but, more generally, that there is no scientific basis for the belief that humans are naturally aggressive and warlike. That belief, however, has not been easily shaken. Among the arguments one sometimes hears are these: Animals are aggressive and we cannot escape the legacy of our evolutionary ancestors; human history is dominated by tales of war and cruelty; and certain areas of the brain and particular hormones are linked to aggression, proving a biological basis for such behavior.
The first thing to be said about animals is that we should be cautious in drawing lessons from them to explain our own behavior, given the mediating force of culture and our capacity for reflection. "Our kinship with other animals does not mean that if their behavior seems often to be under the influence of instincts, this must necessarily also be the case in humans," says an anthropologist. He quotes one authority who has written: "There is no more reason to believe that man fights wars because fish or beavers are territorial than to think that man can fly because bats have wings."
Animals are not even as aggressive as some people think -- unless the term "aggression" is stretched to include killing in order to eat. Organized group aggression is rare in other species, and the aggression that does exist is typically a function of the environment in which animals find themselves. Scientists have discovered that altering their environment, or the way they are reared, can have a profound impact on the level of aggression found in virtually all species. Furthermore, animals cooperate -- both within and among species -- far more than many of us assume on the basis of watching nature documentaries.
When we turn to human history, we find an alarming amount of aggressive behavior, but we do not find reason to believe the problem is innate. Here are some of the points made by critics of biological determinism:
Even if a behavior is universal, we cannot automatically conclude it is part of our biological nature. All known cultures may produce pottery, but that doesn't mean there is a gene for pottery making. Other institutions once thought to be natural are now very difficult to find. In a century or two, says a sociologist, "it is possible that people will look back and regard war in much the same way as today we look back at the practice of slavery."
Aggression, in any case, is nowhere near universal. The above-mentioned anthropologist has edited a book, which features accounts of peaceful cultures. It is true that these are hunter-gatherer societies, but the fact that any humans live without violence would seem to refute the charge that we are born aggressive. In fact, cultures that are "closer to nature" would be expected to be the most warlike if the proclivity for war were really part of that nature. Just the reverse seems to be true. Erich Fromm put it this way: "The most primitive men are the least warlike and ...warlikeness grows in proportion to civilization. If destructiveness were innate in man, the trend would have to be the opposite."
Just as impressive as peaceful cultures are those that have become peaceful. In a matter of a few centuries, Sweden has changed from a fiercely warlike society to one of the least violent among industrialized nations. This shift -- like the existence of war itself -- can more plausibly be explained in terms of social and political factors rather than by turning to biology.
While it is indisputable that wars have been fought frequently, the fact that they seem to dominate our history may say more about how history is presented than about what actually happened. "We write and teach our history in terms of violent events, marking time by wars," says a psychologist. "When we don't have wars, we call it the 'interwar years.' It's a matter of selective reporting."
The presence of some hormones or the stimulation of certain sections of the brain has been experimentally linked with aggression. But after describing these mechanisms in some detail, a physiological psychologist emphasizes that aggressive behavior is always linked to an external stimulus. "That is," he says, "even though the neural system specific to a particular kind of aggression is well-activated, the behavior does not occur unless an appropriate target is available...and even then it can be inhibited...."
So important is the role of the environment that talking of an "innate1'tendency to be aggressive makes little sense for animals, let alone for humans. It is as if we were to assert that because there can be no fires without oxygen, and because the Earth is blanketed by oxygen, it is in the nature of our planet for buildings to burn down.
All of this concerns the matter of human aggressiveness in general. The idea that war in particular is biologically determined is even more farfetched "When one country attacks another country, this doesn't happen because people in the country feel aggressive toward those in the other," explains a biologist. "If it were true, we wouldn't need propaganda or a draft: All those aggressive people would sign up right away. State 'aggression' is a matter of political policy, not a matter of feeling."
The point was put well by Jean Jacques Rousseau more than two centuries ago: "War is not a relation between man and man, but between State and State, and individuals are enemies accidentally." That states must "psych up" men to fight makes it even more difficult to argue for a connection between our natures and the fact of war. In the case of the nuclear arms race, this connection is still more tenuous. Says Bernard Lown, cochairman of International Physicians for the Prevention of Nuclear War, which received the Nobel Peace Prize in 1985: "The individual's behavior, whether he's aggressive or permissive or passive, is not the factor that makes up his outlook toward genocide. Even the person who's aggressive won't readily accept extinction."

★人種や民族(感)
 【1】人種や民族、国民といった特殊な同一性は、歴史的に不安定な同一性であることを、まず、確認しておこう。そして、自由な競争が世界の人々に富をもたらすという新自由主義の掲げるバラ色の未来像と同じように、国民擁護のために地球化に反対するべきだといった発想も、私は非現実的であると考えている。
 【2】なぜなら、狭義の地球化による貧富の差の拡大と階層化とを促進する新自由主義的な論理と、国民主義・人種主義は、同時進行しているからである。国民主義擁護ではない地球化批判の作業にとって、国民国家と地球化の共犯関係の歴史的考察が欠かせないのはこのためである。
 【3】地球化の最近の進展は職業労働を再編し、習慣や感性の在り方を革命的に変更し、これまで用いられてきた国民統合のための制度や政策を時代遅れにしてしまった。
 例えば、国民教育という制度。「均質な国民」を作るための教育という考え方。【4】そして「均質な国民」すべてが身につけることを期待された「国民文化」という理念。国民教育の頂点としての大学の自己正当化の基盤にあったこの理念は、世界の各地で失効しつつある。大学や教育の根本的な見直しが叫ばれているのは日本だけではない。
 【5】といって、社会における学歴の重要性が失われたわけではない。個人の社会・経済的上昇可能性を予測する上で、階級的出自、人種、性別などの因子に比べて、学歴の占める位置がますます大きくなってきている。【6】学歴社会化は世界的な趨勢になっており、地球化の顕著な特徴である。そこで、自己責任と実力主義という新自由主義的な修辞によって、学歴社会化が正当化されようとしている。
 【7】しかし、高学歴を持たない労働者の市場は縮小しつつあり、雇用の危機に絶えず脅かされる低学歴層の人々が大量に社会的に分離されマイノリティー(少数派)化する危険のまえで「国民」文化の欺瞞性がありありと見えてきたのである。∵
 【8】かつて国民国家は、均質な国民が共有する「国民文化」という未来に向けた理念と、不平等と抗争という現在の現実を古代から連続する民族的同一性という過去の虚構において総合することによって、人々から進歩への膨大なエネルギーを喚起する強力な社会的装置であった。
 【9】しかし、差別と格差を絶えず産み出す地球化は国民の内と外を問わず進行する。ところが国民という範疇は社会的経済的格差を「外」と「内」の違いに置き換え、人々に不安を与える複雑な社会的現実に国民の「内」と「外」の比喩による単純で安直な因果的説明を与えるのだ。
 【0】地球化による分断・階層化への最もシニカルな対処は、国民の統合を強調し、国民の団結を語ることによって錯綜した問題を「外」から「内」へと転位することだろう。国歌や国旗といった国民統合の古びた技術に回帰しようとする動きがあるのも、国民の団結によって地球化のもたらす諸問題が解決するはずだという、思い込みが広がっているからだろう。あたかも「日本人が一丸となれは」問題が解消してしまうかのように。
 地球化への実効性のある批判の第一歩は、人種や国民という同一性への依拠が地球化への対抗になるという感傷的な期待を絶つことである。そして、人々の境遇の地球的、歴史的独自性をあくまで尊重しつつ、地球化によって逆境に追い込まれた人々との連携を倫理的にまた知的に、地球的に模索することである。

(酒井直樹の新聞論説より)